Listing 1 - 8 of 8 |
Sort by
|
Choose an application
The interaction between stress and fear conditioning serves as an illuminating model in the understanding of anxiety disorders. Previous work has shown that stress may change subsequent fear acquisition and extinction learning, likely through the stress hormone cortisol. However, variable effects have been described, ranging from facilitative, inhibitory, to null effects. In this article, we first performed a systematic review on the association of stress on subsequent fear acquisition and extinction learning in men. Then, we performed secondary analysis on an existing dataset comprising 133 men who underwent the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST) prior to completing a fear acquisition and extinction task. We examined whether cortisol response trajectories derived from latent-class growth analysis (mild responders (n = 15), moderately-low responders (n = 46), moderately-high responders (n = 48), and hyper responders (n = 24)) could predict the rate of fear acquisition and/or extinction learning, as assessed by US expectancy ratings and skin conductance responses (SCRs). Our systematic review did not reveal a clear consensus pertaining to the association of stress on subsequent fear acquisition. For fear extinction, a negative association of stress exposure, and thus greater extinction resistance, was indicated. In our analysis, we found no significant associations between cortisol response trajectories in response to the MAST and subsequent fear acquisition. We did find an association with extinction learning as assessed by US expectancy ratings, suggesting better safety learning in participants with a hyper-responder cortisol trajectory participant. In addition, during extinction learning, higher overall SCRs were observed in these hyper-responders, indicating greater general arousal during extinction learning independent of stimulus type. These results demonstrate that cortisol responsiveness to a psychosocial stressor interferes with rate of extinction learning at the subjective level and promoted general arousal during this phase at the physiological level. As extinction learning is the theoretical model of exposure therapy, these findings could have clinical implications.
Choose an application
The microbiota-gut-brain (MGB) axis is a bidirectional communication system with multiple signaling routes between the microbiota, the gut and the brain. Evidence suggests that the gut and the brain can influence another, and the MGB axis is being investigated as a link between stress sensitivity and gastrointestinal disorders. An important signaling route of the MGB-axis is via the immune system, which can transmit inflammatory signals bidirectionally between the gut and the brain. This signaling route represents a potential mechanism through which stress-induced inflammation and gut inflammation may be linked, highlighting the intricate interplay between psychological stress, the immune system, and gut health. Although previous studies in specific populations have demonstrated a correlation between systemic inflammation and stress-related outcomes, evidence for an association in the general population remains limited. Given the increasing recognition of the importance of the MGB axis in mental health, the relationship between gut inflammation, systemic inflammation, and stress-related outcomes should be explored. The aim of this thesis was to investigate the association between gut and systemic inflammation with subjective stress, mood indices, and vagus nerve activity in the general population. A cross-sectional study was conducted and preliminary analyses included 37 participants (81.08% female) who were recruited between October 2022 and April 2023. Included participants had a mean age of 28.2 years (SD = 9.86), and a mean body mass index (BMI) of 23.9 kg/m2 (SD = 3.84). Stool samples were collected to measure fecal calprotectin levels as a marker of gut inflammation, while high-sensitive C-reactive protein (hs-CRP) was measured in serum as an indicator of systemic inflammation. Participants completed psychological questionnaires to evaluate subjective stress and affective states, and electrocardiogram (ECG) recordings were obtained to assess resting-state heart rate variability (HRV) as a measurement of vagus nerve activity. Simple linear regression and general linear models (GLM) were employed to assess the association between fecal calprotectin, hs-CRP and stress outcomes, including HRV and psychological questionnaires. Participants were grouped according to their fecal calprotectin level (below quantification (<4mg/kg), low (4-12 mg/kg), low-to-moderate (13-50 mg/kg)) and assessed for group differences in stress outcomes. Participants in the low-to-moderate fecal calprotectin group had higher hs-CRP levels compared to those in the low fecal calprotectin group (p = 0.0195), but caution is warranted, as the association may be BMI-dependent. We did not detect significant associations between hs-CRP and physiological questionnaires and we were unable to replicate previously established significant associations between hs-CRP levels and HRV indices. Participants in the low-to-moderate fecal calprotectin group had lower State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) state scores than participants in the low fecal calprotectin group (p = 0.0164). There was no significant association between fecal calprotectin and HRV indices. In conclusion, these findings suggest a complex relationship between gut and systemic inflammation, subjective stress, and vagus nerve activity in the general population. Further research is needed to confirm these preliminary findings and elucidate their underlying mechanisms.
Choose an application
The gut microbiota and the brain are known to influence each other bi-directionally through complex pathways – the microbiota-gut-brain axis (MGBA). This relationship can be studied by altering the gut composition with gut-modulating interventions like antibiotics. Preliminary work has shown that rifaximin, a non-systemic antibiotic, may possess stress-reducing effects by attenuating psychophysiological responses. However, the majority of the available data come from rodent studies or underpowered human studies. Thus, we aimed to investigate the impact of rifaximin on psychophysiological responses to stress in healthy men. We hypothesized that a two-week treatment with rifaximin would result in the attenuation of psychophysiological stress-related responses through gut microbiota alterations. To this end, we performed a randomized, triple-blinded, placebo-controlled trial where each participant took either 550 mg rifaximin or placebo, twice per day for two weeks. Pre- and post-intervention, physiological stress responses were compared by measuring cardiovascular parameters like pulse rate (PR), systolic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) before, during and after an acute stress task, the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST). Concurrently, psychological stress responses were obtained through visual analogue scale (VAS) ratings at similar timepoints. A total of thirty-four participants received either rifaximin or placebo (ngroup 1 = 15; ngroup 2 = 19; treatment groups remain blinded until the completion of the trial). Age (mean = 26.59; SD = 6.06) and BMI (mean = 22.12; SD = 1.99) were not significantly different between groups. The MAST successfully induced psychobiological stress responses in both treatment groups at pre- and post-intervention as demonstrated by significant increases in PR, SB, DBP, and VAS ratings of stress, discomfort, and pain during the height of the MAST (main effect of timepoint, all p < 0.05). However, the lack of group-by-visit and group-by-visit-by-timepoint interactions indicates that the intervention did not differentially modulate these parameters (all p > 0.05). This study on half of our target sample, although underpowered, has demonstrated that rifaximin does not alter psychophysiological responses to stress compared to placebo. It is possible that rifaximin affects other stress-related pathways such as the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the immune pathway. Thus, additional parameters should be considered for future research such as stress hormone levels (cortisol and adrenaline) and inflammatory markers (cytokines, C-reactive protein). Furthermore, the effect of rifaximin on the modulation of gut microbiota should be further analyzed through fecal analysis (e.g. bacterial count, alpha and beta diversity, and metagenomics), and gut microbiota metabolites such as short-chain fatty acids (SCFAs), should be quantified as a potential mediator.
Choose an application
The global increase in depression represents a significant public health burden, emphasizing the need to identify modifiable risk factors for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Over the past two decades, evidence has suggested systemic inflammation as a potential risk factor. Crucially, gut inflammation and disruptions in gut microbiota composition have been shown to influence systemic inflammation. It is hypothesized that dietary interventions could positively impact this disruption and through the microbiota-gut-brain (MGB) axis impact depression-related-inflammation. However, results from previous studies investigating this relationship are inconsistent or may lack important mediators. Specifically, a fiber-rich diet may offer therapeutic benefits for depression by promoting the production of short-chain fatty acids (SCFAs), known for their anti-inflammatory properties. Therefore, this thesis aimed to explore the immune-modulating effects of dietary fiber on stress and affective state as an endophenotype for depression in the general population and the effect of putative mediators SCFAs, gut inflammation and systemic inflammation. A cross-sectional study involving 83 participants from the general Belgian population was conducted, utilizing the Perceived Stress Scale, 10-items (PSS-10), and momentary measures of stress, positive affect (PA), and negative affect (NA) as indicators of mental well-being. Serum hs-CRP levels served as a marker of systemic inflammation, while fecal calprotectin levels indicated gut inflammation. Daily dietary fiber intake was assessed using a Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ), and fecal SCFA levels were measured as a representative marker of colonic concentrations. Spearman correlations and mediation analysis were performed to assess direct and indirect effects. Our findings did not reveal significant associations between dietary fiber intake, stress and affective state, aligning with inconsistent findings in existing literature on this relationship. Interestingly, we observed a significant negative association between hs-CRP and acute momentary stress (p = 0.042) and NA (p < 0.0001). However, caution is needed in interpreting this result due to the absence of controlled confounders. Furthermore, mediation analysis did not identify significant indirect effects, but a direct negative effect of dietary fiber on momentary NA (95% CI [-0.50; -0.0031]). Future research should include taxonomic analyses of the microbiome and serum SCFAs with a larger sample size to deepen our understanding of the relationship between dietary fiber and depressive symptoms and possible mediators.
Choose an application
Anxiety disorders are a growing public health concern, characterized by excessive fear responses. Treatments are often based on exposure therapy and extinction learning. Pharmacological interventions have proven to be effective to enhance exposure therapy outcome and improve extinction learning, however, fear often returns. Other factors that could improve extinction learning, such as diet, are therefore gaining interest. One nutrient that has appeared important for extinction learning is alpha-linolenic acid (ALA), however research on its acute effects is sparse. The present study is a follow-up of the study by Miller et al. (2018). They found that acute consumption of a milkshake supplemented with walnut oil, a rich source of ALA, resulted in faster and more profound fear extinction in a predictive learning task compared to an equicaloric amount of cream (saturated fat) or a baseline with no added fat (control). They found no effects of the milkshake for the other phases of the learning task (habituation, acquisition and generalization). This study examined the differential effects of four milkshakes containing different nutrients on the performance of a fear-based predictive learning task. The milkshakes contained cream (saturated fat), walnut oil (alpha linolenic acid), cream with orange juice (saturated fat with flavonoids) or maltodextrin (glucose). A total of 128 participants were semi-randomly assigned to one of the four conditions. Participants could not consume anything with calories, nicotine or caffeine three hours before the experiment. They filled in mood, hunger and milkshake related questions and completed interim activities to allow for digestion and absorption of the milkshakes. To finish, participants completed the fear-based predictive learning task. Body temperature was measured to accurately replicate the previous study by Miller et al. (2018). Based on the previous findings by Miller et al. (2018), it was hypothesized that walnut oil would show more profound and rapid extinction compared to cream. Studies found that consumption of orange juice, due to its anti-inflammatory compounds, and glucose could positively influence learning ability. Therefore, faster extinction was also expected for the cream with orange juice and maltodextrin milkshake compared to the cream milkshake. No effects were expected for the habituation, acquisition and generalization phase of the predictive learning task. Results showed that consumption of walnut oil resulted in faster, but not more profound, extinction on the fear-based learning task, compared to cream. In addition, the cream condition was significantly outperformed by maltodextrin, orange juice and walnut oil. Equal performance between conditions was found for habituation, acquisition and generalization. These findings suggest that walnut oil is favorable for faster extinction, but in the end extinction was equally complete over all conditions. Also, no condition-related differences were observed regarding positive and negative state affect. Results could implicate that consumption of ALA is a beneficial addition to exposure therapy in the treatment of fear- and anxiety-related pathology.
Choose an application
Introduction: The gut microbiome has been identified as a key player in the gut-brain axis (GBA), affecting our psychological functioning, particularly stress sensitivity. To investigate this relationship, the gut microbiome can be manipulated, including with the aid of antibiotics such as rifaximin. Previous studies have demonstrated that rifaximin intervention reduces depressive-like behaviour in rats and mitigates psychological stress responses in a small number of healthy volunteers. However, most evidence about the microbiome GBA has been studied in rodents, with limited studies conducted in humans. Therefore, our study aims to investigate whether administering rifaximin can improve stress sensitivity in healthy men. Methods: Each participant was administered either 550 mg rifaximin or placebo twice daily for 2 weeks. All participants were subjected to the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST) at preand post-intervention. Cardiovascular parameters, including systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and pulse rate (PR) were measured at 12 time points before, during, and after the MAST, along with subjective stress responses using visual analogue scales (VAS) and State-Trait Anxiety inventory (STAI). The 12 time points were reduced to peak, peak-baseline, and peak-end for SBP, DBP, PR and VAS. High sensitive C-reactive protein (hs-CRP), a marker for inflammation, was measured at 4 time points before and after the MAST from which data was reduced to end-baseline. In addition to the primary endpoints, The root mean square of the successive differences (RMSSD) between heart rates was measured as an index for resting heart rate variability (HRV). Furthermore, emotional states were evaluated using affective questionnaires, including GAD-7, PHQ-9, PSS, GSRS, PANAS, LEIDS-R, and BFI10. Finally, macronutrient intake and the Bristol Stool Score (BSS) were assessed as well. Results: Group 1 consisted of 2 men whereas the median age was 32 and median BMI was 21.298, while Group 2 comprised 5 men whereas the median age was 22 and the median BMI was 22.635. In both groups, there was no significant difference observed in macronutrient intake, BSS, and affective questionnaires at post-intervention compared to pre-intervention. Resting HRV was increased at post-intervention compared to pre-intervention in Group 2, but without significant difference in both groups. In terms of the response to the MAST, only Group 2 exhibited reductions in peak (SBP), peak-baseline (SBP and DBP), and peak-end (SBP and DBP) at post-intervention compared to pre-intervention. However, none of the results were statistically significant. Furthermore, no significant difference in PR, subjective stress, and hsCRP was found in both groups at post-intervention compared to pre-intervention. Discussion: This study did not offer more insight into the relation between rifaximin intervention and stress sensitivity in healthy men as none of the parameters reported a significant difference at post-intervention. More work with a larger study population will need to be performed. Gut microbiome analysis and short-chain fatty acids measurements could provide more information about the mechanism of rifaximin-induced gut microbiota alterations on stress sensitivity. Other indicators to measure stress, such as brain activity, salivary cortisol, Interleukin-6, and HRV need to be taken into account in future research.
Choose an application
Short-chain fatty acids (SCFAs), the main metabolites produced in the colon by bacterial fermentation of dietary fiber, are thought to play a key role in microbiota-gut-brain communication. Previous research has shown that administering SCFAs to the large intestine can reduce the cortisol response to acute laboratory stress, suggesting that SCFAs may modulate HPA-axis reactivity to psychosocial stress. Furthermore, acute stress tends to impair executive function (EF), a term that refers to the internally driven, top-down, goal-directed control of cognition and behavior. The current study is designed to examine EF as a functional outcome of attenuating the cortisol response to acute laboratory stress by colonic SCFA delivery. We will focus on three core EFs: working memory, response inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. Stress generally impairs working memory and cognitive flexibility, but enhances response inhibition. Since the study did not begin during my time in the lab, my thesis aimed to address several unknowns within the study design through three pilot studies. The first pilot study examined whether there are practice effects when participants repeatedly perform cognitive tasks to measure core EF. Fourteen participants completed a battery of executive function tasks (n-back task, stop signal task, Wisconsin card sorting task) at three timepoints separated by 2 and 4 weeks. No significant differences in performance were observed across timepoints, but ceiling effects and suboptimal data output led to adjustments in the cognitive tasks before moving on to the next pilot. In the second pilot study, core EF was measured after the induction of acute stress using the Maastricht Acute Stress Test (MAST) and compared with EF in no-stress conditions using a sham version of the MAST. Although subjective and physiological stress responses were significantly higher after administration of the MAST, no significant differences in performance were observed between stress and no-stress conditions for any of the tasks. An extension of this pilot study is planned to account for the fact that we could not counterbalance the stress and sham conditions across the two study visits, which prevented us from observing potential stress-related changes in EF. The third and final pilot study evaluated the functionality of SCFA colon delivery capsules by establishing an in vitro dissolution profile. The capsules exhibited a delayed opening, indicating that they were not suitable for accurate delivery of SCFAs to the colon. Together, these pilot studies provide important insights to improve the initial study design and ensure that the study proceeds nicely once it is ready to start.
Choose an application
According to the World Health Organization (WHO), 1 in 13 globally suffers from anxiety. Anxiety disorders are common and pervasive mental disorders, yet they are hard to treat. Lately, there has been a growing interest in the role of the microbiota-gut-brain axis in mental disorders, more specifically, in anxiety disorders. Alterations in the gut microbiota can exert influences on the gut-brain axis and one specific group of microbial metabolites, short chain fatty acids (SCFAs) are speculated to play a dominant role in this interaction. Thus, understanding these interactions may be of great importance regarding treatment and prevention. Over the past years, researches tried to manipulate mice in order to develop suitable models and make a translation to human research possible. However, the complexity of these interactions makes it very hard to translate these findings from mice to humans. This study aimed to answer the question if SCFAs could influence fear in humans. More specifically, we hypothesized a decrease in fear expression across conditioning, extinction, recall and renewal. Furthermore, we expected a decline in self-reported negative mood and distress. These hypotheses were tested in a double blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. A pre-posttest design with a one-week intervention of SCFAs was performed in three conditions (placebo, SCFA equivalent to 10gr of fiber, SCFAs equivalent to 20gr of fiber) during which participants adhered to a low-fiber diet. 66 healthy male participants were included in the study. The aim was to look at the physiological role of SCFAs by looking at the site of production, using colon delivery capsules. Participants engaged in a fear task on two test days, which measured fear processes such as acquisition, extinction learning, extinction recall and fear renewal. Negative mood and distress were assessed using self-reported questionnaires. Linear mixed models were used to examine if the intervention influenced the different phases and trials of fear learning across test days. Additionally, to investigate differences in subjective ratings of fear, participants’ affect ratings of the fear task were explored. This study found no evidence for the hypotheses that SCFAs can decrease fear reaction across acquisition, extinction learning, extinction recall and fear renewal in healthy humans. Furthermore, no evidence was found for a decline in self-reported mood and distress. However, translating animal models to human models is difficult for several reasons. With this study being one of the first making the translation to healthy humans, it offers useful insights for further research. Furthermore, research in animals proposed an important role for SCFAs in fear, suggesting that further research in humans should be considered.
Listing 1 - 8 of 8 |
Sort by
|